Search This Blog

CCE in brief

My photo
Recovering backpacker, Cornwallite at heart, political enthusiast, catalyst, writer, husband, father, community volunteer, unabashedly proud Canadian. Every hyperlink connects to something related directly or thematically to that which is highlighted.

Wednesday, 20 March 2013

The Bias Against Creativity (Jennifer S. Mueller)


Explains a lot, doesn't it?
 




Jennifer S. Mueller


University of Pennsylvania


Shimul Melwani


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill


Jack A. Goncalo


Cornell University



 
 


Abstract


People often reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as a desired goal. To explain this paradox, we

propose that people can hold a bias against creativity that is not necessarily overt, and which is activated when

people experience a motivation to reduce uncertainty. In two studies, we measure and manipulate uncertainty

using different methods including: discrete uncertainty feelings, and an uncertainty reduction prime. The

results of both studies demonstrated a negative bias toward creativity (relative to practicality) when

participants experienced uncertainty. Furthermore, the bias against creativity interfered with participants’

ability to recognize a creative idea. These results reveal a concealed barrier that creative actors may face as they

attempt to gain acceptance for their novel ideas.


Keywords


creativity, bias, goals


Comments

Suggested Citation


Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2011).


The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject


creative ideas



[Electronic version]. Retrieved [insert date], from Cornell University, ILR School site:


http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/450/


Required Publisher Statement


Copyright held by


Sage Publications. Final version forthcoming as: Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A.


(in press). The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas.


Psychological Science.


Reprinted with permission. All rights reserved.


This article is available at DigitalCommons@ILR:


http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/450


Bias Against Creativity

1

Running head: BIAS AGAINST CREATIVITY

The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas

Jennifer S. Mueller

University of Pennsylvania

Shimul Melwani

University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill

Jack A. Goncalo

Cornell University

Keywords: Creativity, bias

**In press at Psychological Science.

Acknowledgements: This idea behind this paper was inspired by Barry Staw’s chapter, “Why No

One Really Wants Creativity.” We would also like to thank the following people for their

insights and help in developing this paper: Jeff Lowenstein, Matthew Cronin and Jennifer

Whitson.

Bias Against Creativity

2

Abstract

People often reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as a desired goal. To explain

this paradox, we propose that people can hold a bias against creativity that is not necessarily

overt, and which is activated when people experience a motivation to reduce uncertainty. In two

studies, we measure and manipulate uncertainty using different methods including: discrete

uncertainty feelings, and an uncertainty reduction prime. The results of both studies

demonstrated a negative bias toward creativity (relative to practicality) when participants

experienced uncertainty. Furthermore, the bias against creativity interfered with participants’

ability to recognize a creative idea. These results reveal a concealed barrier that creative actors

may face as they attempt to gain acceptance for their novel ideas.

Bias Against Creativity

3

Do people desire creative ideas? Most scholars would propose that the answer to this

question is an obvious ‘yes,’ asserting that creativity is the engine of scientific discovery

(Hennessey & Amabile, 2010), the fundamental driving force of positive change (George, 2007),

and associated with intelligence, wisdom, and moral goodness (Niu & Sternberg, 2006;

Sternberg, 1985). However, while people strongly endorse this positive view of creativity,

scholars have long been puzzled by the finding that organizations, scientific institutions, and

decisions-makers routinely reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as an important

goal (Ford & Gioia, 2000; Staw, 1995; West, 2002). Similarly, research documents that teachers

dislike students who exhibit curiosity and creative thinking even though teachers acknowledge

creativity as an important educational goal (Dawson, D'Andrea, Affinito, & Westby, 1999;

Runco, 1989; Westby & Dawson, 1995). We offer a new perspective to explain this puzzle. Just

as people have deeply-rooted biases against people of a certain age, race or gender that are not

necessarily overt (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995), so too can people hold deeply-rooted negative

views of creativity that are not openly acknowledged. Revealing the existence and nature of a

bias against creativity can help explain why people might reject creative ideas and stifle

scientific advancement, even in the face of strong intentions to the contrary.

Creative ideas are both novel and useful (Hennessey & Amabile, 2010), and novelty is

the key distinguishing feature of creativity beyond ideas that are merely well done (Amabile,

Barsade, Mueller, & Staw, 2005). Yet the requirement that creative ideas contain novelty can

also promote a tension in evaluators’ minds when they judge whether to pursue an idea. Indeed,

evaluators have a hard time viewing novelty and practicality as attributes that go hand in hand,

often viewing them as inversely related (Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2009). There are several

reasons why. Practical ideas are generally valued (Sanchez-Burks, 2005). However, the more

Bias Against Creativity

4

novel an idea, the more uncertainty can exist about whether an idea is practical, useful, error free,

and reliably reproduced (Amabile, 1996). When endorsing a novel idea, people can experience

failure (Simonton, 1984), perceptions of risk (Rubenson & Runco, 1995), social rejection when

expressing the idea to others (Moscovici, 1976; Nemeth, 1986), and uncertainty about when their

idea will reach completion (Metcalfe, 1986). Uncertainty is an aversive state (Fiske & Taylor,

1991; Heider, 1958) which people feel a strong motivation to diminish and avoid (Whitson &

Galinsky, 2008). Hence, people can also have negative associations with novelty; an attribute at

the heart of what makes ideas creative in the first place.

Although the positive associations with creativity are typically the focus of attention both

among scholars and practitioners, the negative associations may also be activated when people

evaluate a creative idea. For example, research on associative thinking suggests that strong

uncertainty feelings may make the negative attributes of creativity, particularly those related to

uncertainty, more salient (Bower, 1981).

This evaluative process is not necessarily overt, making the bias against creativity

potentially insidious. In fact, there is often strong normative pressure to endorse creative ideas

(Flynn & Chatman, 2001) and a strong social desirability bias against expressing any view of

creativity as negative (Runco, 2010). This resulting state is similar to that identified in research

on racial bias; a conflict between an explicit preference towards creativity and unacknowledged

negative associations with creativity (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). In other words, uncovering a

bias against creative ideas requires a method more subtle than simply asking directly. Therefore,

we decided to employ a measure that assesses explicit attitudes in addition to implicit attitudes

which are less susceptible to self-presentation biases and normative pressures (Greenwald,

Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009). In two studies, we test whether uncertainty measured and

Bias Against Creativity

5

manipulated in two different ways, promotes a greater bias against creativity relative to

practicality. In the second study we investigate whether this bias deters peoples’ ability to

recognize creative ideas.


EXPERIMENT 1

Method


Participants and Design


Participants (N = 73) were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: uncertainty (n =

28) or baseline (n = 45). 51% were men (mean age= 22.74 years). Each participant took an


implicit


attitude test (IAT) as well as an explicit attitude test to assess their bias against creativity


relative to practicality.


Procedure and Materials


Participants in the uncertainty condition were told that they might receive additional

payment based on a random lottery (not performance). Participants in the baseline condition

were not given the opportunity to receive extra money. A pilot study (N = 82) verified that the

uncertainty manipulation evoked significantly higher uncertainty feelings than a baseline

condition. All participants took an openness to experience inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992), a

trait which is highly related to creativity (Feist, 1998).

Participants’ automatic mental associations with creativity versus practicality were

assessed using the Implicit Association Test (IAT) (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998).

This measure relies on test-takers' speed of response to represent the strength of their mental

associations. The IAT measures participants’ reaction times when rating pairings between an

attitude object (e.g., creativity or practicality) and an evaluative dimension (e.g., good or bad). In

the computerized version of the IAT, this pairing is achieved by using the keyboard (say, a left

Bias Against Creativity

6

key) to be pressed in response to items from the two paired categories, creativity+bad, while

another key (say, the right key) is pressed for the other pair, practicality+good. The speed at

which this pairing is completed compared to opposite pairing is interpreted as a measure of the

strength of the implicit evaluation. Our IAT used words that reflected creativity (e.g.

novel,


creative


, inventive, original) versus practicality (e.g. practical, functional, constructive, and


useful


). In addition our IAT used words that reflected good (rainbow, cake, sunshine, laughter,


peace, heaven


) versus bad (vomit, hell, agony, rotten, poison, ugly). The block order was


counterbalanced such that half of the participants performed the creative + good component first,

whereas the other half performed the creative + bad component first. The IAT effect was formed

by subtracting response latencies for the creative + good task from the creative + bad tasks. We

scored the IAT using the

D statistic (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003), a method less


influenced by procedural variables, such as order or counterbalancing, as well as cognitive

ability (Cai, Sriram, Greenwald, & McFarland, 2004). The implicit bias score was calculated by

subtracting creativity from practicality attitudes; higher values indicate more bias against

creativity relative to practicality.

Participants also rated their

explicit positive and negative associations with creativity and


practicality. Specifically, participants rated their attitudes towards creativity and practicality on a

7-point scale ranging from 1=strongly negative, 4 = neutral, and 7= strongly positive.

Participants assessed attitudes towards creativity (e.g., creative, inventive, original, and novel;

alpha = .77), and practicality (e.g., practical, functional, constructive, useful; alpha = .88).

Participants indicated positive associations (i.e., above the scale mid-point) with both creativity

(M=5.37, SD=.75) and practicality (M=5.43, SD=.91). Explicit bias was calculated by

subtracting creativity from practicality associations (M=.06, SD=.91).

Bias Against Creativity

7


Results and Discussion


Table 1 shows descriptives for all major variables. An ANCOVA controlling for

openness to experience revealed no significant differences in

explicit bias when comparing the


high (

M=.02, SD=.83) and low uncertainty conditions (M=-.11, SD=.96), F(1, 70)= .07, P=.78.


However a second ANCOVA also controlling for openness to experience revealed that

participants in the uncertainty condition showed an

implicit bias against creativity relative to


practicality (

M=.15, SD=.54) which significantly differed from participants in the baseline


condition who showed an implicit bias in favor of creativity relative to practicality (

M=-.23, SD=


.47),

F(1, 70)= 13.13, P=.001; condition accounted for 11% of the variance in implicit bias.


Discussion


Experiment 1 shows that people hold ambivalent attitudes towards creativity. While

participants in the baseline condition evidenced positive implicit associations with creativity

relative to practicality, participants in the uncertainty condition exhibited an implicit bias against

creativity relative to practicality. In Experiment 2 we wished to extend these findings to show

that the motivation to reduce uncertainty when problem solving can activate the creativity bias.

Specifically, scholars propose that effective creative problem solving includes both generating

many novel options and subsequently reducing uncertainty by identifying the single best option

from the set (Cropley, 2006). We propose that this latter orientation towards identifying the

optimal solution may prime an uncertainty reduction motive or intolerance for uncertainty and

thereby evoke the creativity bias. Additionally, we explore whether the creativity bias might also

deter the recognition of a creative idea.


EXPERIMENT 2

Method


Bias Against Creativity

8


Participants and Design


140 undergraduate students (55% female; mean age= 20.66) were randomly assigned to

one of two conditions: high tolerance for uncertainty (n = 70) and low tolerance for uncertainty

(n = 70).


Procedure and Materials


Participants in the high tolerance for uncertainty condition were told to write an essay

supporting the statement, “For every problem, there is more than one correct solution” while

those in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition were asked to write an essay supporting the

statement, “For every problem, there is only one correct solution.” A three item manipulation

check assessed uncertainty when evaluating an idea (e.g., “I feel uncertain about this idea),”

anchors from 1 = not at all, 7 = very much so (alpha = .78). Participants in the low tolerance

condition were significantly more uncertain (

M=4.36, SD=1.23) than those in the high tolerance


condition (

M= 3.87, SD=1.33; F(1, 133)=5.14, P=.025). After being exposed to the experimental


manipulation, each participant took the same implicit and explicit creativity-practicality bias tests

used in Experiment 1. Subsequently, participants were asked to rate a creative idea which we

pre-tested using a different sample of undergraduates (N = 36) who rated this idea (a running

shoe with nanotechnology that adjusted fabric thickness to cool the foot and reduce blisters) as

being highly creative (M=5.82, SD=.80), novel (M=5.62, SD=1.02), and practical (M=5.85,

SD=.92) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1=not at all to 7=extremely so. Before exposure to the

manipulation, participants also took the openness to experience inventory.

Participants rated the idea using the creativity scale, employing the same six synonyms

for creativity used in both the implicit and explicit bias tests (M=5.41, SD=1.05, alpha=.78).


Results and Discussion


Bias Against Creativity

9

Table 2 shows descriptives for all major variables. An ANCOVA controlling for

openness to experience revealed that participants in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition

were not significantly different in their level of explicit bias against creativity (

M= .20, SD=.81)


as compared to participants in the high tolerance condition (

M= .22, SD=.94), F(1, 133)= .14,


P=.71. However, a second ANCOVA controlling for openness to experience revealed that

participants in the low uncertainty tolerance condition were more

implicitly biased against


creativity relative to practicality (

M= .07, SD=.43) than participants in the high uncertainty


tolerance condition (M

= -.16, SD=.46), F(1, 133)= 7.87, P=.007, who exhibited positive


associations with creativity relative to practicality. A third ANCOVA controlling for openness to

experience identified that participants in the low tolerance condition rated the idea as less

creative (

M= 5.06, SD=1.06) than participants in the high tolerance condition (M= 5.76,


SD=.93),

F(1, 137)= 15.48, P=.000.


A hierarchical regression showed that the relationship between experimental condition

and creativity ratings (

B = -.64, t (134) = -3.81, p < .001) became less significant when including


implicit bias in the model (

B = -.56, t (134) = -3.30, p < .01). A 95% bootstrapped confidence


interval of the indirect effect of condition on creativity ratings through implicit bias did not

include zero [-.24, -.02], demonstrating partial mediation (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Mediation

analyses controlled for both explicit bias and openness to experience at each step indicating that

relatively low levels of uncertainty tolerance led to higher levels of the implicit bias that in turn

contributed to lower ratings of creativity controlling for participants’ explicit bias and general

openness to experience.


Discussion


Bias Against Creativity

10

Experiment 2 both replicated the finding that uncertainty promotes negative associations

with creativity relative to practicality, and extended this finding by showing that the bias against

creativity interfered with participants’ ability to recognize a creative idea.


GENERAL DISCUSSION


Robert Goddard, the father of modern rocket propulsion, endured ridicule and derision

from his contemporary scientific peers who stated his ideas were ludicrous and impossible. This

example is not unique, and would puzzle creativity theorists as research shows that expert raters

who are themselves creative are even more likely to accurately recognize and assess creativity

(Hennessey, Amabile, & Mueller, 2010; Runco & Smith, 1992). Our results show that regardless

of how open minded people are, when they feel motivated to reduce uncertainty either because

they have an immediate goal of reducing uncertainty, or feel uncertain generally, this may bring

negative associations with creativity to mind which result in lower evaluations of a creative idea.

Our findings imply a deep irony. Prior research shows that uncertainty spurs the search for and

generation of creative ideas (Audia & Goncalo, 2007; Tiedens & Linton, 2001), yet our findings

reveal that uncertainty also makes us less able to recognize creativity, perhaps when we need it

most.

Beyond merely having a preference for the status quo or familiar ideas (Eidelman,

Crandall, & Pattershall, 2009; Zajonc, 2001), our results suggest that people have ambivalent

feelings towards creativity. On one hand, participants in the baseline and uncertainty tolerance

conditions demonstrated

positive implicit associations with creativity relative to practicality.


Additionally, 95% of participants in the high uncertainty and uncertainty intolerance conditions

rated their

explicit attitudes towards creativity as positive- higher than ‘4’ the mid-point of a 7-


point scale- and statistically equivalent to practicality. On the other hand, the implicit measure

Bias Against Creativity

11

identified that participants in each high uncertainty condition associated words like “vomit,”

“poison,” and “agony,” more so with creativity than practicality. Because there is such a strong

social norm to endorse creativity and people also feel authentic positive attitudes towards

creativity, people may be reluctant to admit that they do not want creativity; hence, the bias

against creativity may be particularly slippery to diagnose. The implicit measures may have

picked up negative associations with creativity under conditions of uncertainty because the

methodology is more resistant to social desirability bias (Greenwald et al., 2009).

If people hold an implicit bias against creativity, then we cannot assume that

organizations, institutions or even scientific endeavors will desire and recognize creative ideas

even when they explicitly state they want them. This is because when journals extol creative

research, universities train scientists to promote creative solutions, R&D companies commend

the development of new products, pharmaceutical companies praise creative medical

breakthroughs, they may do so in ways that promote uncertainty by requiring gate-keepers to

identify the single “best” and most “accurate” idea thereby creating an unacknowledged aversion

to creativity. In addition, our results suggest that if people have difficulty gaining acceptance for

creative ideas especially when more practical and unoriginal options are readily available, the

field of creativity may need to shift its current focus from identifying how to generate more

creative ideas to identifying how to help innovative institutions recognize and accept creativity.

Future research should identify factors which mitigate or reverse the bias against creativity.

Bias Against Creativity

12


REFERNCES


Amabile, T. M. (1996).

Creativity in context: Update to "The Social Psychology of Creativity."


(Vol. 317). Boulder, CO, US: Westview Press.

Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at

work.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 367-403.


Audia, P., & Goncalo, J. (2007). Past success and creativity over time: A study of inventors in

the hard disk drive industry.

Management Science, 53(1), 1.


Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.

American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148.


Cai, H., Sriram, N., Greenwald, A. G., & McFarland, S. G. (2004). The Implicit Association

Test's "D" Measure can Minimize a Cognitive Skill Confound: Comment on McFarland

and Crouch (2002).

Social Cognition, 22(6), 673-684.


Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO

Personality Inventory.

Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5-13.


Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking.

Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391-


404.

Dawson, V. L., D'Andrea, T., Affinito, R., & Westby, E. L. (1999). Predicting creative behavior:

A reexamination of the divergence between traditional and teacher-defined concepts of

creativity.

Creativity Research Journal, 12(1), 57-66.


Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., & Pattershall, J. (2009). The existence bias.

Journal of Personality


and Social Psychology, 97


(5), 765-775.


Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity.

Personality


and Social Psychology Review, 2


(4), 290-309.


Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991).

Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill


Bias Against Creativity

13

Flynn, F. J., & Chatman, J. A. (2001). Strong cultures and innovation: Oxymoron or

opportunity? . In T. Holden, N. Sparrow & W. Starbuck (Eds.),

International Handbook


of Organizational Culture and Climate


(pp. 263-287). Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.


Ford, C. M., & Gioia, D. A. (2000). Factors Influencing Creativity in the Domain of Managerial

Decision Making.

Journal of Management, 26(4), 705-732.


Gaertner, S., & Dovidio, J. (Eds.). (1986).

The aversive form of racism. New York: Academic


Press.

George, J. M. (2007). Chapter 9: Creativity in Organizations.

The Academy of Management


Annals, 1


(1), 439 - 477.


Greenwald, A., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit

cognition: The implicit association test.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,


74


, 1464-1480.


Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and

stereotypes.

Psychological Review, 102(1), 4-27.


Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit

Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm.

Journal of Personality and Social


Psychology, 85


(2), 197-216.


Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and

using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity.

Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 97


(1), 17-41.


Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity.

Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569-


598.

Bias Against Creativity

14

Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M., & Mueller, J. S. (2010). Chapter 46: Consensual Assessment.

. In

Encyclopedia of Creativity, 4th edition.


Metcalfe, J. (1986). Premonitions of insight predict impending error.

Journal of Experimental


Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12


(4), 623-634.


Moscovici, S. (1976).

Social influence and social change. London: UK: Academic Press.


Nemeth, C. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence.

Psychological


Review, 93


(1), 23-32.


Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. (2006). The philosophical roots of Western and Eastern conceptions of

creativity.

Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. Vol, 26(1-2), 18-38.


Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects

in simple mediation models.

Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers.


Special Issue: Web-based archive of norms, stimuli, and data: Part 2, 36


(4), 717-731.


Rietzschel, E., Nijstad, B., & Stroebe, W. (2009). The selection of creative ideas after individual

idea generation: Choosing between creativity and impact.

British Journal of Psychology,


0


(1-23).


Rubenson, D. L., & Runco, M. A. (1995). The psychoeconomic view of creative work in groups

and organizations.

Creativity and Innovation Management, 4(4), 232-241.


Runco, M. A. (1989). Parents' and teachers' ratings of the creativity of children.

Journal of Social


Behavior & Personality, 4


(1), 73-83.


Runco, M. A. (2010). Creativity has no dark side. In D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman

& M. A. Runco (Eds.),

The Dark Side of Creativity. New York, NY: Cambridge


University Press.

Bias Against Creativity

15

Runco, M. A., & Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative

ideas.

Personality and Individual Differences, 13(3), 295-302.


Sanchez-Burks, J. (2005). Protestant relational ideology: The cognitive underpinnings and

organizational implications of an American anomaly.

Research in organizational


behavior, 26


, 265-305.


Simonton, D. K. (1984).

Genius, Creativity, and Leadership: Histriometric Inquiries. Boston,


MA: Harvard University Press

Staw, B. M. (1995). Why no one really wants creativity. In C. Ford & D. A. Gioia (Eds.),


Creative Action in Organizations: Ivory Tower Visions and Real World Voices


. Thousand


Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom.

Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 49


(3), 607-627.


Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The

effects of specific emotions on information processing.

Journal of Personality and Social


Psychology, 81


(6), 973-988.


West, M. A. (2002). Ideas are ten a penny: It's team implementation not idea generation that

counts.

Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51(3), 411-424.


Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the classroom?

Creativity


Research Journal, 8


(1), 1-10.


Whitson, J., & Galinsky, A. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception.


Science, 322


(5898), 115.


Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal.

Current Directions in


Psychological Science, 10


(6), 224-228.


Bias Against Creativity

16

Bias Against Creativity

17

Table 1. Descriptives for all major variables used in Experiment 1, N = 73

1


Mean SD 1 2 3

1. Openness to Experience 4.01 .65

2. Condition ( 1 = uncertainty, 0 = baseline) .38 .43 .11

3. Explicit Bias .06 .91 -.32** -.07

4. Implicit Bias -.09 .53 -.25** .35** .29*

*p < .05; **p < .01


1


uncertainty condition contained 28 participants and the baseline condition contained 45


participants

Bias Against Creativity

18

Table 2. Descriptives of all major variables used in Experiment 2, N = 140

1


Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5

1. Openness to Experience 5.59 .99

2. Condition ( 1 = low tolerance for uncertainty, 0 = high

tolerance for uncertainty)

.50 .50 -.08

3. Uncertainty Feelings When Evaluating an Idea 4.12 1.30 -.04 .20*

4. Explicit Bias .21 .87 -.23** -.01 .01

5. Implicit Bias -.05 .46 -.34** .25** -.13 .20*

6. Creativity Rating 5.41 1.05 .20* -.33** -.01 -.24** -.33**

*p < .05; **p < .01


1


70 participants were in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition, and 70 participants were in the high tolerance for uncertainty

condition

No comments:

Post a Comment