Explains a lot, doesn't it?
Jennifer S. Mueller
University of Pennsylvania
Shimul Melwani
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Jack A. Goncalo
Cornell University
Abstract
People often reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as a desired goal. To explain this paradox, we
propose that people can hold a bias against creativity that is not necessarily overt, and which is activated when
people experience a motivation to reduce uncertainty. In two studies, we measure and manipulate uncertainty
using different methods including: discrete uncertainty feelings, and an uncertainty reduction prime. The
results of both studies demonstrated a negative bias toward creativity (relative to practicality) when
participants experienced uncertainty. Furthermore, the bias against creativity interfered with participants’
ability to recognize a creative idea. These results reveal a concealed barrier that creative actors may face as they
attempt to gain acceptance for their novel ideas.
Keywords
creativity, bias, goals
Comments
Suggested Citation
Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2011).
The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject
creative ideas
[Electronic version]. Retrieved [insert date], from Cornell University, ILR School site:
http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/450/
Required Publisher Statement
Copyright held by
Sage Publications. Final version forthcoming as: Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A.
(in press). The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas.
Psychological Science.
Reprinted with permission. All rights reserved.
This article is available at DigitalCommons@ILR:
http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/450
Bias Against Creativity
1
Running head: BIAS AGAINST CREATIVITY
The bias against creativity: Why people desire but reject creative ideas
Jennifer S. Mueller
University of Pennsylvania
Shimul Melwani
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Jack A. Goncalo
Cornell University
Keywords: Creativity, bias
**In press at Psychological Science.
Acknowledgements: This idea behind this paper was inspired by Barry Staw’s chapter, “Why No
One Really Wants Creativity.” We would also like to thank the following people for their
insights and help in developing this paper: Jeff Lowenstein, Matthew Cronin and Jennifer
Whitson.
Bias Against Creativity
2
Abstract
People often reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as a desired goal. To explain
this paradox, we propose that people can hold a bias against creativity that is not necessarily
overt, and which is activated when people experience a motivation to reduce uncertainty. In two
studies, we measure and manipulate uncertainty using different methods including: discrete
uncertainty feelings, and an uncertainty reduction prime. The results of both studies
demonstrated a negative bias toward creativity (relative to practicality) when participants
experienced uncertainty. Furthermore, the bias against creativity interfered with participants’
ability to recognize a creative idea. These results reveal a concealed barrier that creative actors
may face as they attempt to gain acceptance for their novel ideas.
Bias Against Creativity
3
Do people desire creative ideas? Most scholars would propose that the answer to this
question is an obvious ‘yes,’ asserting that creativity is the engine of scientific discovery
(Hennessey & Amabile, 2010), the fundamental driving force of positive change (George, 2007),
and associated with intelligence, wisdom, and moral goodness (Niu & Sternberg, 2006;
Sternberg, 1985). However, while people strongly endorse this positive view of creativity,
scholars have long been puzzled by the finding that organizations, scientific institutions, and
decisions-makers routinely reject creative ideas even when espousing creativity as an important
goal (Ford & Gioia, 2000; Staw, 1995; West, 2002). Similarly, research documents that teachers
dislike students who exhibit curiosity and creative thinking even though teachers acknowledge
creativity as an important educational goal (Dawson, D'Andrea, Affinito, & Westby, 1999;
Runco, 1989; Westby & Dawson, 1995). We offer a new perspective to explain this puzzle. Just
as people have deeply-rooted biases against people of a certain age, race or gender that are not
necessarily overt (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995), so too can people hold deeply-rooted negative
views of creativity that are not openly acknowledged. Revealing the existence and nature of a
bias against creativity can help explain why people might reject creative ideas and stifle
scientific advancement, even in the face of strong intentions to the contrary.
Creative ideas are both novel and useful (Hennessey & Amabile, 2010), and novelty is
the key distinguishing feature of creativity beyond ideas that are merely well done (Amabile,
Barsade, Mueller, & Staw, 2005). Yet the requirement that creative ideas contain novelty can
also promote a tension in evaluators’ minds when they judge whether to pursue an idea. Indeed,
evaluators have a hard time viewing novelty and practicality as attributes that go hand in hand,
often viewing them as inversely related (Rietzschel, Nijstad, & Stroebe, 2009). There are several
reasons why. Practical ideas are generally valued (Sanchez-Burks, 2005). However, the more
Bias Against Creativity
4
novel an idea, the more uncertainty can exist about whether an idea is practical, useful, error free,
and reliably reproduced (Amabile, 1996). When endorsing a novel idea, people can experience
failure (Simonton, 1984), perceptions of risk (Rubenson & Runco, 1995), social rejection when
expressing the idea to others (Moscovici, 1976; Nemeth, 1986), and uncertainty about when their
idea will reach completion (Metcalfe, 1986). Uncertainty is an aversive state (Fiske & Taylor,
1991; Heider, 1958) which people feel a strong motivation to diminish and avoid (Whitson &
Galinsky, 2008). Hence, people can also have negative associations with novelty; an attribute at
the heart of what makes ideas creative in the first place.
Although the positive associations with creativity are typically the focus of attention both
among scholars and practitioners, the negative associations may also be activated when people
evaluate a creative idea. For example, research on associative thinking suggests that strong
uncertainty feelings may make the negative attributes of creativity, particularly those related to
uncertainty, more salient (Bower, 1981).
This evaluative process is not necessarily overt, making the bias against creativity
potentially insidious. In fact, there is often strong normative pressure to endorse creative ideas
(Flynn & Chatman, 2001) and a strong social desirability bias against expressing any view of
creativity as negative (Runco, 2010). This resulting state is similar to that identified in research
on racial bias; a conflict between an explicit preference towards creativity and unacknowledged
negative associations with creativity (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). In other words, uncovering a
bias against creative ideas requires a method more subtle than simply asking directly. Therefore,
we decided to employ a measure that assesses explicit attitudes in addition to implicit attitudes
which are less susceptible to self-presentation biases and normative pressures (Greenwald,
Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009). In two studies, we test whether uncertainty measured and
Bias Against Creativity
5
manipulated in two different ways, promotes a greater bias against creativity relative to
practicality. In the second study we investigate whether this bias deters peoples’ ability to
recognize creative ideas.
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
Participants and Design
Participants (N = 73) were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: uncertainty (n =
28) or baseline (n = 45). 51% were men (mean age= 22.74 years). Each participant took an
implicit
attitude test (IAT) as well as an explicit attitude test to assess their bias against creativity
relative to practicality.
Procedure and Materials
Participants in the uncertainty condition were told that they might receive additional
payment based on a random lottery (not performance). Participants in the baseline condition
were not given the opportunity to receive extra money. A pilot study (N = 82) verified that the
uncertainty manipulation evoked significantly higher uncertainty feelings than a baseline
condition. All participants took an openness to experience inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992), a
trait which is highly related to creativity (Feist, 1998).
Participants’ automatic mental associations with creativity versus practicality were
assessed using the Implicit Association Test (IAT) (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998).
This measure relies on test-takers' speed of response to represent the strength of their mental
associations. The IAT measures participants’ reaction times when rating pairings between an
attitude object (e.g., creativity or practicality) and an evaluative dimension (e.g., good or bad). In
the computerized version of the IAT, this pairing is achieved by using the keyboard (say, a left
Bias Against Creativity
6
key) to be pressed in response to items from the two paired categories, creativity+bad, while
another key (say, the right key) is pressed for the other pair, practicality+good. The speed at
which this pairing is completed compared to opposite pairing is interpreted as a measure of the
strength of the implicit evaluation. Our IAT used words that reflected creativity (e.g.
novel,
creative
, inventive, original) versus practicality (e.g. practical, functional, constructive, and
useful
). In addition our IAT used words that reflected good (rainbow, cake, sunshine, laughter,
peace, heaven
) versus bad (vomit, hell, agony, rotten, poison, ugly). The block order was
counterbalanced such that half of the participants performed the creative + good component first,
whereas the other half performed the creative + bad component first. The IAT effect was formed
by subtracting response latencies for the creative + good task from the creative + bad tasks. We
scored the IAT using the
D statistic (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003), a method less
influenced by procedural variables, such as order or counterbalancing, as well as cognitive
ability (Cai, Sriram, Greenwald, & McFarland, 2004). The implicit bias score was calculated by
subtracting creativity from practicality attitudes; higher values indicate more bias against
creativity relative to practicality.
Participants also rated their
explicit positive and negative associations with creativity and
practicality. Specifically, participants rated their attitudes towards creativity and practicality on a
7-point scale ranging from 1=strongly negative, 4 = neutral, and 7= strongly positive.
Participants assessed attitudes towards creativity (e.g., creative, inventive, original, and novel;
alpha = .77), and practicality (e.g., practical, functional, constructive, useful; alpha = .88).
Participants indicated positive associations (i.e., above the scale mid-point) with both creativity
(M=5.37, SD=.75) and practicality (M=5.43, SD=.91). Explicit bias was calculated by
subtracting creativity from practicality associations (M=.06, SD=.91).
Bias Against Creativity
7
Results and Discussion
Table 1 shows descriptives for all major variables. An ANCOVA controlling for
openness to experience revealed no significant differences in
explicit bias when comparing the
high (
M=.02, SD=.83) and low uncertainty conditions (M=-.11, SD=.96), F(1, 70)= .07, P=.78.
However a second ANCOVA also controlling for openness to experience revealed that
participants in the uncertainty condition showed an
implicit bias against creativity relative to
practicality (
M=.15, SD=.54) which significantly differed from participants in the baseline
condition who showed an implicit bias in favor of creativity relative to practicality (
M=-.23, SD=
.47),
F(1, 70)= 13.13, P=.001; condition accounted for 11% of the variance in implicit bias.
Discussion
Experiment 1 shows that people hold ambivalent attitudes towards creativity. While
participants in the baseline condition evidenced positive implicit associations with creativity
relative to practicality, participants in the uncertainty condition exhibited an implicit bias against
creativity relative to practicality. In Experiment 2 we wished to extend these findings to show
that the motivation to reduce uncertainty when problem solving can activate the creativity bias.
Specifically, scholars propose that effective creative problem solving includes both generating
many novel options and subsequently reducing uncertainty by identifying the single best option
from the set (Cropley, 2006). We propose that this latter orientation towards identifying the
optimal solution may prime an uncertainty reduction motive or intolerance for uncertainty and
thereby evoke the creativity bias. Additionally, we explore whether the creativity bias might also
deter the recognition of a creative idea.
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Bias Against Creativity
8
Participants and Design
140 undergraduate students (55% female; mean age= 20.66) were randomly assigned to
one of two conditions: high tolerance for uncertainty (n = 70) and low tolerance for uncertainty
(n = 70).
Procedure and Materials
Participants in the high tolerance for uncertainty condition were told to write an essay
supporting the statement, “For every problem, there is more than one correct solution” while
those in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition were asked to write an essay supporting the
statement, “For every problem, there is only one correct solution.” A three item manipulation
check assessed uncertainty when evaluating an idea (e.g., “I feel uncertain about this idea),”
anchors from 1 = not at all, 7 = very much so (alpha = .78). Participants in the low tolerance
condition were significantly more uncertain (
M=4.36, SD=1.23) than those in the high tolerance
condition (
M= 3.87, SD=1.33; F(1, 133)=5.14, P=.025). After being exposed to the experimental
manipulation, each participant took the same implicit and explicit creativity-practicality bias tests
used in Experiment 1. Subsequently, participants were asked to rate a creative idea which we
pre-tested using a different sample of undergraduates (N = 36) who rated this idea (a running
shoe with nanotechnology that adjusted fabric thickness to cool the foot and reduce blisters) as
being highly creative (M=5.82, SD=.80), novel (M=5.62, SD=1.02), and practical (M=5.85,
SD=.92) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1=not at all to 7=extremely so. Before exposure to the
manipulation, participants also took the openness to experience inventory.
Participants rated the idea using the creativity scale, employing the same six synonyms
for creativity used in both the implicit and explicit bias tests (M=5.41, SD=1.05, alpha=.78).
Results and Discussion
Bias Against Creativity
9
Table 2 shows descriptives for all major variables. An ANCOVA controlling for
openness to experience revealed that participants in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition
were not significantly different in their level of explicit bias against creativity (
M= .20, SD=.81)
as compared to participants in the high tolerance condition (
M= .22, SD=.94), F(1, 133)= .14,
P=.71. However, a second ANCOVA controlling for openness to experience revealed that
participants in the low uncertainty tolerance condition were more
implicitly biased against
creativity relative to practicality (
M= .07, SD=.43) than participants in the high uncertainty
tolerance condition (M
= -.16, SD=.46), F(1, 133)= 7.87, P=.007, who exhibited positive
associations with creativity relative to practicality. A third ANCOVA controlling for openness to
experience identified that participants in the low tolerance condition rated the idea as less
creative (
M= 5.06, SD=1.06) than participants in the high tolerance condition (M= 5.76,
SD=.93),
F(1, 137)= 15.48, P=.000.
A hierarchical regression showed that the relationship between experimental condition
and creativity ratings (
B = -.64, t (134) = -3.81, p < .001) became less significant when including
implicit bias in the model (
B = -.56, t (134) = -3.30, p < .01). A 95% bootstrapped confidence
interval of the indirect effect of condition on creativity ratings through implicit bias did not
include zero [-.24, -.02], demonstrating partial mediation (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Mediation
analyses controlled for both explicit bias and openness to experience at each step indicating that
relatively low levels of uncertainty tolerance led to higher levels of the implicit bias that in turn
contributed to lower ratings of creativity controlling for participants’ explicit bias and general
openness to experience.
Discussion
Bias Against Creativity
10
Experiment 2 both replicated the finding that uncertainty promotes negative associations
with creativity relative to practicality, and extended this finding by showing that the bias against
creativity interfered with participants’ ability to recognize a creative idea.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Robert Goddard, the father of modern rocket propulsion, endured ridicule and derision
from his contemporary scientific peers who stated his ideas were ludicrous and impossible. This
example is not unique, and would puzzle creativity theorists as research shows that expert raters
who are themselves creative are even more likely to accurately recognize and assess creativity
(Hennessey, Amabile, & Mueller, 2010; Runco & Smith, 1992). Our results show that regardless
of how open minded people are, when they feel motivated to reduce uncertainty either because
they have an immediate goal of reducing uncertainty, or feel uncertain generally, this may bring
negative associations with creativity to mind which result in lower evaluations of a creative idea.
Our findings imply a deep irony. Prior research shows that uncertainty spurs the search for and
generation of creative ideas (Audia & Goncalo, 2007; Tiedens & Linton, 2001), yet our findings
reveal that uncertainty also makes us less able to recognize creativity, perhaps when we need it
most.
Beyond merely having a preference for the status quo or familiar ideas (Eidelman,
Crandall, & Pattershall, 2009; Zajonc, 2001), our results suggest that people have ambivalent
feelings towards creativity. On one hand, participants in the baseline and uncertainty tolerance
conditions demonstrated
positive implicit associations with creativity relative to practicality.
Additionally, 95% of participants in the high uncertainty and uncertainty intolerance conditions
rated their
explicit attitudes towards creativity as positive- higher than ‘4’ the mid-point of a 7-
point scale- and statistically equivalent to practicality. On the other hand, the implicit measure
Bias Against Creativity
11
identified that participants in each high uncertainty condition associated words like “vomit,”
“poison,” and “agony,” more so with creativity than practicality. Because there is such a strong
social norm to endorse creativity and people also feel authentic positive attitudes towards
creativity, people may be reluctant to admit that they do not want creativity; hence, the bias
against creativity may be particularly slippery to diagnose. The implicit measures may have
picked up negative associations with creativity under conditions of uncertainty because the
methodology is more resistant to social desirability bias (Greenwald et al., 2009).
If people hold an implicit bias against creativity, then we cannot assume that
organizations, institutions or even scientific endeavors will desire and recognize creative ideas
even when they explicitly state they want them. This is because when journals extol creative
research, universities train scientists to promote creative solutions, R&D companies commend
the development of new products, pharmaceutical companies praise creative medical
breakthroughs, they may do so in ways that promote uncertainty by requiring gate-keepers to
identify the single “best” and most “accurate” idea thereby creating an unacknowledged aversion
to creativity. In addition, our results suggest that if people have difficulty gaining acceptance for
creative ideas especially when more practical and unoriginal options are readily available, the
field of creativity may need to shift its current focus from identifying how to generate more
creative ideas to identifying how to help innovative institutions recognize and accept creativity.
Future research should identify factors which mitigate or reverse the bias against creativity.
Bias Against Creativity
12
REFERNCES
Amabile, T. M. (1996).
Creativity in context: Update to "The Social Psychology of Creativity."
(Vol. 317). Boulder, CO, US: Westview Press.
Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at
work.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 50, 367-403.
Audia, P., & Goncalo, J. (2007). Past success and creativity over time: A study of inventors in
the hard disk drive industry.
Management Science, 53(1), 1.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.
American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148.
Cai, H., Sriram, N., Greenwald, A. G., & McFarland, S. G. (2004). The Implicit Association
Test's "D" Measure can Minimize a Cognitive Skill Confound: Comment on McFarland
and Crouch (2002).
Social Cognition, 22(6), 673-684.
Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO
Personality Inventory.
Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5-13.
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking.
Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391-
404.
Dawson, V. L., D'Andrea, T., Affinito, R., & Westby, E. L. (1999). Predicting creative behavior:
A reexamination of the divergence between traditional and teacher-defined concepts of
creativity.
Creativity Research Journal, 12(1), 57-66.
Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., & Pattershall, J. (2009). The existence bias.
Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 97
(5), 765-775.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity.
Personality
and Social Psychology Review, 2
(4), 290-309.
Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991).
Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill
Bias Against Creativity
13
Flynn, F. J., & Chatman, J. A. (2001). Strong cultures and innovation: Oxymoron or
opportunity? . In T. Holden, N. Sparrow & W. Starbuck (Eds.),
International Handbook
of Organizational Culture and Climate
(pp. 263-287). Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.
Ford, C. M., & Gioia, D. A. (2000). Factors Influencing Creativity in the Domain of Managerial
Decision Making.
Journal of Management, 26(4), 705-732.
Gaertner, S., & Dovidio, J. (Eds.). (1986).
The aversive form of racism. New York: Academic
Press.
George, J. M. (2007). Chapter 9: Creativity in Organizations.
The Academy of Management
Annals, 1
(1), 439 - 477.
Greenwald, A., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit
cognition: The implicit association test.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74
, 1464-1480.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and
stereotypes.
Psychological Review, 102(1), 4-27.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit
Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85
(2), 197-216.
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and
using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 97
(1), 17-41.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity.
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569-
598.
Bias Against Creativity
14
Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M., & Mueller, J. S. (2010). Chapter 46: Consensual Assessment.
. In
Encyclopedia of Creativity, 4th edition.
Metcalfe, J. (1986). Premonitions of insight predict impending error.
Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12
(4), 623-634.
Moscovici, S. (1976).
Social influence and social change. London: UK: Academic Press.
Nemeth, C. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence.
Psychological
Review, 93
(1), 23-32.
Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. (2006). The philosophical roots of Western and Eastern conceptions of
creativity.
Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. Vol, 26(1-2), 18-38.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects
in simple mediation models.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers.
Special Issue: Web-based archive of norms, stimuli, and data: Part 2, 36
(4), 717-731.
Rietzschel, E., Nijstad, B., & Stroebe, W. (2009). The selection of creative ideas after individual
idea generation: Choosing between creativity and impact.
British Journal of Psychology,
0
(1-23).
Rubenson, D. L., & Runco, M. A. (1995). The psychoeconomic view of creative work in groups
and organizations.
Creativity and Innovation Management, 4(4), 232-241.
Runco, M. A. (1989). Parents' and teachers' ratings of the creativity of children.
Journal of Social
Behavior & Personality, 4
(1), 73-83.
Runco, M. A. (2010). Creativity has no dark side. In D. H. Cropley, A. J. Cropley, J. C. Kaufman
& M. A. Runco (Eds.),
The Dark Side of Creativity. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Bias Against Creativity
15
Runco, M. A., & Smith, W. R. (1992). Interpersonal and intrapersonal evaluations of creative
ideas.
Personality and Individual Differences, 13(3), 295-302.
Sanchez-Burks, J. (2005). Protestant relational ideology: The cognitive underpinnings and
organizational implications of an American anomaly.
Research in organizational
behavior, 26
, 265-305.
Simonton, D. K. (1984).
Genius, Creativity, and Leadership: Histriometric Inquiries. Boston,
MA: Harvard University Press
Staw, B. M. (1995). Why no one really wants creativity. In C. Ford & D. A. Gioia (Eds.),
Creative Action in Organizations: Ivory Tower Visions and Real World Voices
. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 49
(3), 607-627.
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The
effects of specific emotions on information processing.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81
(6), 973-988.
West, M. A. (2002). Ideas are ten a penny: It's team implementation not idea generation that
counts.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51(3), 411-424.
Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the classroom?
Creativity
Research Journal, 8
(1), 1-10.
Whitson, J., & Galinsky, A. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception.
Science, 322
(5898), 115.
Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal.
Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 10
(6), 224-228.
Bias Against Creativity
16
Bias Against Creativity
17
Table 1. Descriptives for all major variables used in Experiment 1, N = 73
1
Mean SD 1 2 3
1. Openness to Experience 4.01 .65
2. Condition ( 1 = uncertainty, 0 = baseline) .38 .43 .11
3. Explicit Bias .06 .91 -.32** -.07
4. Implicit Bias -.09 .53 -.25** .35** .29*
*p < .05; **p < .01
1
uncertainty condition contained 28 participants and the baseline condition contained 45
participants
Bias Against Creativity
18
Table 2. Descriptives of all major variables used in Experiment 2, N = 140
1
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5
1. Openness to Experience 5.59 .99
2. Condition ( 1 = low tolerance for uncertainty, 0 = high
tolerance for uncertainty)
.50 .50 -.08
3. Uncertainty Feelings When Evaluating an Idea 4.12 1.30 -.04 .20*
4. Explicit Bias .21 .87 -.23** -.01 .01
5. Implicit Bias -.05 .46 -.34** .25** -.13 .20*
6. Creativity Rating 5.41 1.05 .20* -.33** -.01 -.24** -.33**
*p < .05; **p < .01
1
70 participants were in the low tolerance for uncertainty condition, and 70 participants were in the high tolerance for uncertainty
condition
No comments:
Post a Comment